Communication

Communication is behavior that influences the actions of other individuals. It consists of displays or signals that can be perceived by other individuals and which convey information to them. Natural selection shapes displays or signals into systems of communication if the transmission of information benefits both the sender and the receiver. Thus, the ultimate cause of communication is the selective advantage it gives to individuals that engage in it. The courtship displays of a male, for example, benefit the male if they attract females. The displays also benefit the female if they allow her to assess whether the male is of the right species and whether he is strong, vigorous, and has other attributes that will make him a good father. A common mutual benefit of communication is the reduction of uncertainty about the status or intentions of the signaler. Even in aggressive interactions, reducing uncertainty helps both sender and receiver to avoid physical harm.

Studies of communication can be complex because they must take into account the sender, the receiver, and the environment. The displays or signals that an animal can generate depend on its physiology and anatomy. Likewise, an animal's ability to perceive displays or signals depends on its sensory physiology and on the environment through which the display or signal must be transmitted.

Chemical signals are durable

Molecules used for chemical communication between individual animals of the same species are called pheromones. Because of the diversity of their molecular structures, pheromones can communicate very specific messages that contain a great deal of information. The mate attraction pheromone of the female silkworm moth is a good example (see Figure 45.3). Male moths as far as several kilometers downwind are informed by these molecules that a female of their species is sexually receptive. By orienting to the wind direction and following the concentration gradient of the molecules, they can find her.

Territory marking is another example in which detailed information is conveyed by chemical communication (Figure 52.11). Pheromone messages left by mammals such as cats and dogs, for example, can reveal a great deal of information about the signaler: species, individual identity, reproductive status, size (indicated by the height of the message), and when the animal was last in the area (indicated by the strength of the scent).

An important feature of pheromones is that once they are released, they remain in the environment for a long time. By contrast, vocal or visual displays disappear as soon as the animal stops signaling or displaying. The durability of pheromone signals enables them to be used to mark trails, as ants do, to mark territory, or to indicate directionality, as in

Pheromones For Territory
Panthera tigris

52.11 Many Animals Communicate with Pheromones To mark her territory, this female tiger is spraying urine, which contains phero-monal secretions from a scent gland in her hindquarters, onto a tree. Other tigers passing the spot will know that the area is "claimed,"and they will know something about the animal who claimed it.

the case of the moth sex attractant. However, it also means that the message cannot be changed rapidly. This inflexibility makes pheromonal communication unsuitable for a rapid exchange of information.

The chemical nature and the size of a pheromone molecule determine its speed of diffusion. The greater the speed of diffusion, the more rapidly the message gets out and the farther it will travel, but the sooner it will disappear. Trail-marking and territory-marking pheromones tend to be relatively large molecules that diffuse slowly; sex attractants tend to be small molecules that diffuse rapidly.

Visual signals are rapid and versatile but are limited by directionality

Visual signals are easy to produce, come in an endless variety, can be changed rapidly, and clearly indicate the position of the signaler. However, the extreme directionality of visual signals means that they are not the best means of getting the attention of a receiver. The receptors of the receiver must be focused on the signaler, or the message will be missed. Most animals are sensitive to light and can therefore receive visual signals, but sharpness of vision limits the detail that can be transmitted. The complexity of the environment also limits visual communication.

Because visual communication requires light, it is not useful at night or in environments that lack light, such as caves and the ocean depths. Some species have surmounted this constraint by evolving their own light-emitting mechanisms.

Fireflies, for example, use a enzymatic mechanism to create flashes of light. By emitting flashes in species-specific patterns, fireflies can advertise for mates at night.

Fireflies also illustrate how some species can exploit the communication systems of other species. There are predatory species of fireflies that mimic the mating flashes of other species. When an eager suitor approaches the signaling individual, it is eaten. Thus, deception can be part of animal communication systems, just as it is part of human use of language.

Auditory signals communicate well over a distance

Compared with visual communication, auditory communication has advantages and disadvantages. Sound can be used at night and in dark environments. It can go around objects that would interfere with visual signals, so it can be used in complex environments such as forests. It is better than visual signals at getting the attention of a receiver because the receiver does not have to be focused on the signaler for the message to be received.

Like visual signals, sound can provide directional information, as we saw in Chapter 44, as long as the receiver has at least two receptors spaced somewhat apart. By maximizing or minimizing certain features of the sounds they emit, animals can make their location easier or more difficult to determine.

Sound is useful for communicating over long distances. Even though the intensity of sound decreases with distance from the source, loud sounds can be used to communicate over distances much greater than those possible with visual signals. An extreme example is the communication of whales. Some whales, such as the humpback, have complex songs. When these sounds are produced at a certain depth (around 1,000 m), they can be heard hundreds of kilometers away. In this way, humpback whales can locate each other over vast areas of ocean.

Auditory signals cannot convey complex information as rapidly as visual signals can, as is implied by the expression "A picture is worth a thousand words." When individuals are in visual contact, an enormous amount of information is exchanged instantaneously (for example, species, sex, individual identity, reproductive status, level of motivation, dominance, vigor, alliances with other individuals, and so on). Coding that amount of information, with all of its subtleties, as auditory signals would take considerable time, thus increasing the possibility that the communicators could be located by predators.

Tactile signals can communicate complex messages

Communication by touch is common, although not always obvious. Animals in close contact use tactile interactions extensively, especially under conditions that do not favor vi sual communication. When eusocial insects such as ants, termites, or bees meet, they contact each other with their antennae and front legs.

One of the best-studied uses of tactile communication, beginning with the work of Karl von Frisch, is the dance of honeybees. When a forager bee finds food, she returns to the hive and communicates her discovery to her hivemates by dancing in the dark on the vertical surface of the honeycomb. The dance is monitored by other bees, who follow and touch the dancer to interpret the message.

If the food source is more than ~80 meters away from the hive, the bee performs a waggle dance (Figure 52.12), which conveys information about both the distance and the direction of the food source. The bee repeatedly traces out a figure-eight pattern as she runs on the vertical surface. She alternates half-circles to the left and right with vigorous wagging of her abdomen in the short, straight run between turns. The angle of the straight run indicates the direction of the food source relative to the direction of the sun. The speed

Hive

Food source

Hive

Honey comb

Pattern of waggle dance

Food source

Honey comb

Food source

Food source

Waggle Dance Pictures
Pattern of waggle dance

Pattern of waggle dance

52.12 The Waggle Dance of the Honeybee (a) A honeybee runs straight up on the vertical surface of the honeycomb in a dark hive while waggling her abdomen to tell her hive-mates that there is a food source in the direction of the sun. The intensity of the waggle indicates exactly how far the food source is. If the food source were in the opposite direction from the sun, she would orient her waggle runs straight down. (b) When her waggle runs are at an angle from the vertical, the other bees know that the same angle separates the direction of the food source from the direction of the sun.

of the dancing indicates the distance to the food source: The farther away it is, the slower the waggle run.

If the food she has found is less than 80 meters from the hive, the forager performs a round dance, running rapidly in a circle and reversing her direction after each circumference. The odor on her body and the round dance combine tactile and chemical cues: The odor indicates the flower to be looked for, and the dance communicates the fact that the food source is within 100 meters of the hive.

Electric signals can also communicate messages

Some species of fish have evolved the ability to generate electric fields in the water around them by emitting a series of electric pulses (see Chapter 45). These trains of electric pulses can be used for sensing objects in the immediate surroundings, and they can also be used for communication.

An electrode connected to an amplifier and a speaker can be used to "listen" to the signals generated by glass knife fish in a tank. Each individual fish emits a pulse at a different frequency, and the frequency each fish uses relates to its status in the population. Males emit lower frequencies than females. The most dominant male has the lowest frequency, and the most dominant female has the highest frequency. When a new individual is introduced into the tank, the other individuals adjust their frequencies so that they do not overlap with the newcomer's, and the newcomer's signal indicates its position in the hierarchy. In their natural environment—the murky waters of tropical rainforests—these fish can tell the identity, sex, and social position of another fish by its electric signals.

Communication has been a fruitful area for investigating the ultimate causes of behavior and how the resulting adaptations have been shaped by the environment. Next we will return to some studies of proximate causes of behavior to see some examples of how "how" questions can be addressed.

Was this article helpful?

0 0
Essentials of Human Physiology

Essentials of Human Physiology

This ebook provides an introductory explanation of the workings of the human body, with an effort to draw connections between the body systems and explain their interdependencies. A framework for the book is homeostasis and how the body maintains balance within each system. This is intended as a first introduction to physiology for a college-level course.

Get My Free Ebook


Post a comment